WHERNTO: wellnes righton operate

Jonathan Balcombe holds three biology degrees and specializes in the study of ethology with a PhD in this area of study. A leading animal behavior researcher he is also an author and speaker.
Below are excerpts from Alexandra’s excellent article about Jonathan:
“It’s easy to come away with a sense that it’s a hard-struggled life out there, but it’s a life worth living,” observes Dr. Balcombe. He has found that many species share related physiologies and behavior patterns as humans, and are thus able to feel such emotions as pain, pleasure, and passion.
What’s more, Dr. Balcombe firmly believes that animals have many lessons that we humans can learn from. “I am an avid animal watcher… they teach me new things every time I watch them carefully and the more I understand the more there is to learn,” says Balcombe.
This is the link to his own site, jonathan-balcombe.com which contains information about his books, writings, courses etc.
Biographical Profile
Jonathan Balcombe was born in England, raised in New Zealand and Canada, and has lived in the United States since 1987. He has three biology degrees, including a PhD in ethology (the study of animal behavior) from the University of Tennessee, where he studied communication in bats. He has published over 40 scientific papers on animal behavior and animal protection.
He is the author of four books, including Pleasurable Kingdom: Animals and the Nature of Feeling Good, Second Nature: The Inner Lives of Animals, and The Exultant Ark: A Pictorial Tour of Animal Pleasure (pdf).
Formerly Senior Research Scientist with the Physicians Committee for Responsible Medicine, he is currently Chair of the Animal Studies Department with the Humane Society University.
Jonathan lives with his family in Germantown, Maryland. In his spare time he enjoys biking, baking, birdwatching, piano, and trying to understand his two cats.
Links
Jonathan Balcombe
This is the main site and contains information about Jonathan’s books,
writings, courses etc.
MacMillan Speakers: Jonathan
Balcombe
Animal behavior expert Jonathan Balcombe is a passionate advocate for
animals and their living spaces. His highly acclaimed books Pleasurable
Kingdom and Second Nature present animals in a new light and presage a
revolution in the human-animal relationship. His most recent book, The
Exultant Art: A Pictorial Tour of Animal Pleasure (2011), received a
glowing review in the New York Times. A PhD scientist and vegan,
Balcombe’s dynamic message resonates with timely issues that affect
everyone, including climate change, biodiversity, and personal health.
He has given lectures and presentations on six continents.Jonathan
Balcombe was born in England and raised in New Zealand and Canada. He
studied biology at Canada’s York University and Carleton University
before earning a PhD in ethology (animal behavior) from the University
of Tennessee. He has written over 40 scientific papers and book
chapters, and many lay articles on animal behavior and animal
protection.
PCRM: Jonathan Balcombe, Ph.D.,
M.S.
Jonathan Balcombe, Ph.D., M.S., is a research scientist with the
Physicians Committee for Responsible Medicine (PCRM), a nationwide
organization of physicians and laypersons that promotes preventive
medicine, especially good nutrition, and addresses controversies in
modern medicine, including ethical issues in research …
TEM Style: Jonathan Balcombe, ethical
man
TEM is proud to present Jonathan Balcombe (seen here with Lucy, the rat)
as the first-ever feature in our Ethical Men series. Congrats, Jonathan!
New Zealand Center for Human-Animal Studies: Dr Jonathan Balcombe
(USA)
Jonathan Balcombe was born in England, raised in New Zealand and Canada,
and has lived in the United States since 1987. He studied biology at
York University and Carleton University in Canada before earning his
doctorate in ethology (animal behavior) from the University of
Tennessee, studying communication in bats. He has written many
scientific papers and lay-articles on animal behavior, humane education,
and animal research …
Free From Harm: Interview with Jonathan
Balcombe
Author, ethologist and biologist Dr. Jonathan Balcombe is breaking new
ground in our understanding of and appreciation for animals.
OneKind: Jonathan Balcombe on The Exultant
Ark
Jonathan Balcombe is an academic and also a successful author of a
number of popular books about animals emotions: specifically animals'
pleasure.
Blogspot: Jonathan Balcombe
The musings and reflections of a biologist as enthralled with animal
life as he is dissatisfied with humanity’s current relationship to it.
Jonathan Balcombe: A Passionate
Ethologist
An excellent, concise essay about ethologist Dr. Balcombe and some of
his remarkable work.
Humane Society University, a pioneer in the animal studies
movement
Moody octopuses, altruistic bats and honeybees that vote are academic
fodder for Washington’s newest university.
Media Press
Farm animals deserve humane
treatment
(Washington Post)
One often sees the term “extremism” applied to certain acts of animal
activism, but never is it used in reference to the extreme cruelties
routinely carried out by the animal industries themselves [“As
eco-terrorism threat diminishes, governments keep eyes on activists,”
front page, March 11]. To list just a few: We take newborn calves from
their mothers so that the latter may be milked, crowd hens into tiny
cages for egg production, sear off the sensitive beak tips of female
chicks, toss the male chicks into meat-grinders or into bins to
suffocate, and consign pregnant sows to metal-and-concrete stalls in
which they can barely move for weeks.
That these practices remain legal makes them no less cruel. Fortunately, consumers can make the non-extreme decision to refrain from funding factory farming by choosing to purchase alternative products.
Jonathan Balcombe, Germantown
Jonathan Balcombe: ‘Stop being beastly to
hens’
(The Guardian)
Animal behaviour scientist Jonathan Balcombe says that our treatment of
animals remains medieval despite a flood of studies shedding light on
how they experience the world
How to Have Fun Like Monkeys, Whales and
Foxes
(Wired Science)
Balcombe surveys a new generation of studies into animal feelings,
especially animal pleasure. Accompanying the scholarship are photographs
of animals seeming to enjoy themselves: hippos and flying foxes,
zebrafish and sharks, parrots and polar bears, a whole animal kingdom of
pleasure.
Jonathan Balcombe: “Second
Nature”
(Diane Rehm Radio Show)
Humans aren’t the only beings who communicate, feel emotions and have
self-awareness. Drawing on the latest research, an animal behaviorist
explains why people need to change the way they treat other living
creatures.
Videos
Why Vegan? Lessons From An Animal Scientist
As a scientist who chooses a plant-based diet, Jonathan Balcombe is not your typical biologist. In this riveting, richly-illustrated presentation, Balcombe reveals startling new discoveries in the realms of animal cognition and emotional complexity–from optimistic starlings, to choosy fishes, to dogs who object to unfair treatment.
Interview - Jonathan Balcombe
Second Nature: The Inner Lives of Animals
Interview with Jonathan Bascombe author of “Second Nature: The Inner
Lives of Animals” recorded May 29, 2010 in Seattle.
Dr. Jonathan Balcombe on Our Relationship to Animals
Second Nature: The Inner Lives of Animals
“Jonathan Balcombe is a rare being, a scientist who has escaped the
narrow orthodoxies of institutional science, an intelligent human being
who is more than ready to recognize intelligences of other kinds, an
intuitive and empathetic observer who nevertheless does not abandon the
highest standards of intellectual inquiry.”
–from the Forward by J.M Coetzee, winner of the Nobel Prize in
Literature
Dr. Jonathan Balcombe at the 2010 VSDC Life-Affirming Thanksgiving celebration
Jonathan Balcombe as guest speaker at the 2010 Vegetarian Society of DC Life-Affirming Thanksgiving celebration.
Friends for Life (Jonathan Balcombe)
Composed and performed by Jonathan Balcombe.
Dr. Jonathan Balcombe on Individuality in Fish
Second Nature: The Inner Lives of Animals
Bshary, R. “Machiavellian Intelligence in Fishes.” In Fish Cognition and
Behavior , edited by C. Brown et al., pp. 223-242. Oxford:
Wiley-Blackwell, 2006.
Dr. Jonathan Balcombe on Animal Virtue and Fairness
Second Nature: The Inner Lives of Animals
Brosnan, S.F., and F.B.M. de Waal. “Monkeys reject unequal pay.” Nature
425 (2003): 297-299.
Books and Reviews
The Exultant Ark - A Pictorial Tour of Animal Pleasure
Jonathan P. Balcombe
Nature documentaries often depict animal life as a grim struggle for survival, but this visually stunning book opens our eyes to a different, more scientifically up-to-date way of looking at the animal kingdom. In more than one hundred thirty striking images, The Exultant Ark celebrates the full range of animal experience with dramatic portraits of animal pleasure ranging from the charismatic and familiar to the obscure and bizarre. These photographs, windows onto the inner lives of pleasure seekers, show two polar bears engaged in about of wrestling, hoary marmots taking time for a friendly chase, Japanese macaques enjoying a soak in a hot spring, a young bull elk sticking out his tongue to catch snowflakes, and many other rewarding moments. Biologist and best-selling author Jonathan Balcombe is our guide, interpreting the images within the scientific context of what is known about animal behavior. In the end, old attitudes fall away as we gain a heightened sense of animal individuality and of the pleasures that make life worth living for all sentient beings.
The Exultant Ark - A Pictorial Tour of Animal Pleasure
Review in NY Times
True to its subtitle — “A Pictorial Tour of Animal Pleasure” — “The Exultant Ark” showcases surprising, funny, touching, sad, heartwarming pictures by photographers all over the world. Dr. Balcombe’s text is a serious examination of the subject of animal pleasure, a study that “remains nascent and largely neglected in scientific discourse.” But it also delights us along the way with Dr. Balcombe’s observations and examples.
Second Nature: The Inner Lives of Animals
Jonathan P. Balcombe
For centuries we believed that humans were the only ones that mattered. The idea that animals had feelings was either dismissed or considered heresy. Today, that’s all changing. New scientific studies of animal behavior reveal perceptions, intelligences, awareness and social skills that would have been deemed fantasy a generation ago. The implications make our troubled relationship to animals one of the most pressing moral issues of our time. Jonathan Balcombe, animal behaviorist and author of the critically acclaimed Pleasurable Kingdom, draws on the latest research, observational studies and personal anecdotes to reveal the full gamut of animal experiencefrom emotions, to problem solving, to moral judgment. Balcombe challenges the widely held idea that nature is red in tooth and claw, highlighting animal traits we have disregarded until now: their nuanced understanding of social dynamics, their consideration for others, and their strong tendency to avoid violent conflict. Did you know that dogs recognize unfairness and that rats practice random acts of kindness? Did you know that chimpanzees can trounce humans in short-term memory games? Or that fishes distinguish good guys from cheaters, and that birds are susceptible to mood swings such as depression and optimism? With vivid stories and entertaining anecdotes, Balcombe gives the human pedestal a strong shake while opening the door into the inner lives of the animals themselves.
Second Nature: The Inner Lives of Animals
Review by Kim Stallwood
Balcombe’s writings are a carefully crafted balance of academic rigor and empathic wisdom.
Pleasurable Kingdom: Animals and the Nature of Feeling Good
Jonathan P. Balcombe
Pleasurable Kingdom presents new evidence that animals - like humans - enjoy themselves. From birds to baboons, insects to iguanas, animals feel good thanks to play, sex, touch, food, anticipation, comfort, aesthetics, and more. Combining rigorous evidence, elegant argument and amusing anecdotes, leading animal behavior researcher Jonathan Balcombe shows that the possibility of positive feelings in creatures other than humans has important ethical ramifications for both science and society. For more information please visit the author’s website at www.pleasurablekingdom.com
The Use of Animals in Higher Education: Problems, Alternatives, & Recommendations
Jonathan P. Balcombe
The Use of Animals in Higher Education is well-researched and annotated and international in scope, with a primary focus on the US. It addresses secondary education as strongly as higher education, with quite a focus on elementary education as well. (get pdf)
Beyond Animal Research Articles at PCRM
Heart Attack Experiments in
Animals
Heart attacks are virtually unknown in mice and rats, probably because
their natural life spans are just two to three years, their heart
physiology is different from that of humans, and, of course, they avoid
fast-food restaurants. But, unfortunately, some animal experimenters
have nonetheless attempted to artificially induce heart problems in
these hapless creatures.
Animal Research and Mobile Phones: Getting a Bad
Connection
A recent article in the British Medical Journal asks “Where is the
evidence that animal research benefits humans?” The authors examined
systematic reviews of six areas of animal research and presented
evidence that it had not informed human medicine. For two of the six
areas, clinical trials were conducted at the same time as the animal
studies, and for three others, clinical trials went ahead despite
evidence of harm from the animal studies. Overall, the analysis reveals
both a practical and a perceived disconnect between animal and human
studies. The authors conclude that “new animal studies should not be
conducted until…their validity and generalisability to clinical medicine
has been assessed.”
Bone-Breaking
Experiments
Understanding how bones break and heal and developing treatments are all
important and worthwhile medical endeavors. Unfortunately, some
experimenters deliberately break healthy animals’ bones. Each year
dozens of such studies are published; these experiments are conducted on
many different animals, including dogs, rats, mice, rabbits, pigs,
sheep, and goats.
The Misunderstood
Rat
Rats are one of the world’s least understood creatures. Stigmatized as
filthy “pests” for centuries, these inquisitive opportunists are
actually naturally sociable and make excellent companions. New
scientific studies show that there is more to rats than laboratory
supply companies would have us believe.
Animal Smoking
Experiments
Scientists like to joke that smoking is a leading cause of statistics.
It’s an amusing observation, but sadly, when it comes to animal
experimentation, it’s all too true. Despite the failure of numerous
animal studies during the 1950s and 1960s to reveal a clear link between
cigarette smoking and cancer—and despite our established knowledge from
human clinical data that smoking is deadly - smoking experiments on
animals continue.
Animal Smoking Experiments: Part
II
Scientists have known for decades that smoking can have devastating
effects on human health. Yet researchers continue to conduct smoking
experiments on animals. Do such experiments offer new insights? In an
earlier column (Part I), I discussed several recent animal smoking
experiments that appear merely to confirm what is already known from
human studies. To investigate further, I followed up on four of these
animal studies (all published in 2004) using the National Institutes of
Health’s online PubMed database.
The Guinea Pigs Deserve a
Hearing
Many years ago I remember seeing in a physiology textbook two
photographs of a guinea pig’s inner ear, taken under a microscope before
and after exposure to destructively loud noise. The first image showed
the sensitive cochlear hairs arranged in neat rows; in the second they
looked like a stand of trees flattened by a violent hurricane.
Animals Still Used in Motion Sickness
Experiments
If you’ve ever suffered motion sickness, you know that it’s a miserable
feeling. Other animals are also vulnerable, and for at least 50 years
scientists have been subjecting monkeys, dogs, cats, rats and other
species to experiments designed to make them ill.
Migraine Research, Part
1
According the UK-based Migraine Trust Association
(www.migrainetrust.org), migraine is the most common neurological
condition in the developed world … There is no cure, but there are many
available treatments. Despite this, animals bear the brunt of scientific
curiosity about migraine. Cats, monkeys, pigs, guinea pigs, and rats are
all being used in headache research. These experimental subjects are not
suffering from migraines (at least not that we can tell), but their
skulls are opened up and their brains tweaked, probed and drugged in a
parade of experiments designed, it seems, more to advance scientific
careers than useful knowledge.
Migraine Research, Part
II
In last month’s column, I described a small sampling of “migraine”
studies on animals, involving usually terminal, invasive procedures on
unconscious (anesthetized) animals (cats, monkeys, rats, etc.) who have
no way of communicating symptoms. But other researchers are pursuing
very different paths. The following sampling of recent studies
underscores the broad range of human clinical methods available for
advancing our understanding of this important condition …
A Stunning Waste of
Pigs
A recent story appeared in New Scientist concerning the use of stun guns
for immobilizing dangerous persons … The New Scientist report mentioned
that, in response to concerns about human deaths associated with Taser
use, animal tests were being initiated to assess their effects on the
hearts of anesthetized pigs.
Confining Rodents in Laboratory Cages Has Troubling
Consequences
Throughout history, human societies have dealt with criminals by
confining them in small cells. Not only does imprisonment remove the
offender from daily life, but by taking away the inmate’s freedom and
autonomy it constitutes punishment … Animals caged in laboratories find
themselves in comparable circumstances.
Chimeras: Beyond Our Moral
Depth?
The chimeras I learned about as a biology student—obscure,
strange-looking fishes of the ocean depths—are not the same as the ones
now at the center of a debate in medical ethics. These chimeras are made
by man, not by nature. As stem cell technology marches ahead, science
now has the means to create creatures that blur the line between humans
and other animals. The specter of a half-chimp/half-man or a mouse with
a human brain is no longer the stuff of science fiction.
Sarin: Terror in the
Laboratories
Originally developed as a chemical warfare agent in 1938, the nerve gas
sarin became a household word when it was used in a terrorist attack on
the Tokyo subway system in March 1995 (one of three such incidents in
Japan between 1994 and 1998). Long-term health effects on Gulf War
veterans have also been attributed to possible low-level sarin exposure.
Despite the pitfalls of trying to extrapolate across species, animals
have been exposed to sarin in hundreds of laboratory experiments dating
back at least to the late 1950s.
Anorexia Nervosa and Animal
Experiments
Anorexia is a terrible illness that afflicts some two million Americans,
mostly young women. Not surprisingly, there is a concerted effort to
understand and address anorexia and other eating disorders. A search for
“anorexia nervosa” on the NIH’s PubMed online database yielded more than
8,600 studies. Most are human clinical studies—but not all. Many
researchers are being funded to perform experiments on animals, usually
(male) rats. But because rodents don’t spontaneously develop eating
disorders, experimenters must create “animal models” of the condition.
Anorexia Nervosa: The Potential of Nonanimal
Research
In last month’s column, I presented some examples of studies in which
rats are starved and exercised in a futile attempt to model anorexia
nervosa (AN), a uniquely human condition. As I researched these studies,
I also encountered numerous human clinical studies. The following recent
examples illustrate just some of the broad range of research strategies
available for human-based research into eating disorders.
Homeopathy: Healing Some, Harming
Others
My local paper recently profiled the Samueli Institute for Information
Biology (SIIB), a nonprofit medical research organization that
emphasizes complementary medicine, such as acupuncture and homeopathy, a
system of medical practice that treats a disease especially by the
administration of minute doses of a remedy that would in healthy persons
produce symptoms similar to those of the disease. The organization’s
stated mission is strongly geared towards healing, and its goals include
alleviating suffering, enhancing well being, and establishing
sustainable healthcare approaches. So far, so good. Then I noticed that
the article mentioned animal studies.
Homeopathy Research: Efficacy
Studies
Because homeopathy involves giving vanishingly small quantities of
ingredients to patients, side effects are rare and treatments
essentially harmless. That does not mean they are effective, however.
The animal studies I described in last month’s column were trying to
address efficacy, not safety. As the following examples from 2005 show,
researchers have a variety of means for assessing alternative treatments
in humans without resorting to cruel animal studies.
Mice on
Soy
A recent study from the University of Colorado made headlines when it
was reported that a soy-based diet worsened heart disease in male mice
carrying a mutation for hypertrophic cardiomyopathy (HCM).
Depressed Mice Study Is Inhumane and Clinically
Irrelevant
A study published in the prestigious journal Science reported on the
apparent role of a protein (called p11) in fighting depression. However,
most of the depressed subjects in this study weren’t people—they were
mice.
Sudden Infant Death
Syndrome
Sudden Infant Death Syndrome (SIDS) is implicated in about 5,000 to
7,000 infant deaths yearly in the United States … A considerable amount
of this research is in the clinical setting … There are also many animal
studies …
Blogs and Opinions at firstscience.com (selected)
(Unfortunately, the links to these items are unavailable at firstscience.com presently.)
Cultured Sparrows
… one enterprising sparrow had probably stumbled on this available food
source (perhaps by accident while trapped?), and it was adopted by other
birds. Sparrow culture.
Steve Irwin Stung by the Press
… I think Irwin would have objected to those headlines announcing his
death by stingray, in which the ray was said to have “attacked” him. The
animal acted in defense. With its poor word-choice, the press succumbed
to the lure of sensationalism, or maybe just ignorance, and undid some
of the good that Irwin had done.
Empathic Mice
A widely reported recent study documented evidence that house mice show
empathy …
Animal Testing my Patience
A recent BBC news article on the rise in animal use in UK labs contained
the following statement from a pro-vivisection spokesperson. “Animal
testing for medical research is unavoidable.” The person who said this
needs an English lesson.
Bird-Brains Not
A new study of bird behavior
[www.sciencenews.org/articles/20061118/fob6.asp] adds to a burgeoning
body of evidence that birds aren’t the bird-brains they were once
thought to be.
Fish Scale New Heights of Social Sophistication
These studies add to a growing body of evidence that fishes are nothing
like the thoughtless, unfeeling automatons they were once thought to be.
That’s no comfort to anyone concerned about humankind’s voracious
plundering of the world’s fish populations, but nature has her own
agenda, and easing humanity’s conscience isn’t on the list.
One Man’s Waste
We owe a lot to insects for helping to keep the world clean. Were it not
for the flies and beetles, the world would soon be awash with organic
waste, and disease and squalor would run rampant.
Tea: Black is Beautiful
A new study reported in the European Heart Journal finds that adding
milk to one’s tea may blunt its cardiovascular benefits
An Elephant Talks - Are We Listening?
The English intellectual Cyril Connolly once quipped that: “imprisoned
in every fat woman [or man] a thin one is wildly signalling to be let
out.” I find that sentiment rather apt for a teenaged elephant who made
international news last week for having mastered eight words of human
speech
A Negative Review Ain’t Necessarily So
I consider myself fortunate that nearly a year passed before my latest
book (www.pleasurablekingdom.com) received a negative review. BioScience
[Vol. 57(1): Pp. 83-84] has published a review titled “Feelings do not
a science make” by Oxford University biologist Marian Stamp Dawkins
which takes me to task for a variety of perceived mis-steps. A colleague
posted Dawkins’s review on his list-serve and it generated a fusillade
of responses in defense of animal experiences …
Sharp Weapons, Sharp Minds
Recent news of chimpanzees fashioning and using deadly weapons grabbed
headlines last week … Less headline-grabbing was the latest in a series
of elegant studies of food caching and recovery in scrub jays …
It’s Time the Fur Flew
Each time I see someone wearing fur, a little bit of my faith in
humanity dies … Perhaps there’s still hope for the demise of an industry
that should have gone out with the arrival of the loom.
What Does A Rat Know, Anyway?
Those who defend animal research often like to point out that about
ninety percent of the animals used are rodents, namely mice and rats.
The upshot is that we should all breathe a sigh of relief. “Oh, they’re
just rodents. Why should we be concerned?”
A Chimp Haven
Earlier this week I visited a facility for chimpanzees given over to
retirement by research laboratories that no longer want them.
A Dog Named Chester
What is special about this “dog” is that he isn’t the coddled canine
whose ancestors barked at lurking predators and accepted scraps at
stone-age firesides … He is a rodent … When I think of Chester’s cousins
being obliterated for someone’s amusement, I wonder which species is
more noble: ours or his. For all our technology and culture, humankind
has a knack for moral fickleness.
Life, liberty and the pursuit of happiness for apes?
In late April, an Austrian judge denied personhood status and legal
guardianship for 26-year-old Matthias Pan, who was kidnapped as an
infant in Sierra Leone after his mother was shot. Brought to Austria
illegally, Pan was sold to a research laboratory where he lived alone in
a cage and was experimented on for many years before finally being
released to a sanctuary. In her concluding statement, the judge
explained that she never doubted that Pan should be considered a person,
but she did not want to set a precedent that might weaken the case of
humans with legal guardians.
Man Bites Shark, Apes Publish Paper
I recently reviewed a book (Anthrozoos, Vol. 20(1)) with the charmingly
buoyant title of “Killing Animals.” The book’s introduction includes the
sobering news that we humans kill more animals today than we have at any
point in history.
Siding with the Amphibian
… as I stood there looking at this frog and contemplating her grim
outlook in this man’s clutches, empathy for the frog overcame me and I
quickly released her in the grass … Last week my 13-year old daughter
spared another leopard frog through a noble act of conscience. She asked
to use alternatives instead of dissecting a dead leopard frog in her 7th
grade science class.
Blogs from Psychology Today (selected)
Falling for Birds, One at a
Time
Nearly 3,000 Red-winged Blackbirds fell dead from the sky in Beebe,
Arkansas on New Year’s Eve.
From Skinny Bitch to Bill Clinton: The Rise of
Veganism
Have you noticed the word “vegan” lately? If you live in America and you
read a newspaper or popular magazines, the chances are you have. If you
watch television, you probably even know how to pronounce it properly.
Extinction Folly - Review of Paper Tiger: A Visual History of the
Thylacine
Today, rewards offered for a living specimen seem like wishful thinking.
Man vs. Wild, Bear Grylls, and the Violent Nature
Myth
A few months ago, surfing the television, I happened upon a scene in
which a fit-looking young man forcibly removed a rattlesnake from
beneath a shrub and smashed its head in with a rock. As he did so, he
warned of the danger posed by these reptiles. How ironic.
Peter Singer, Vioxx, and the Future of Animal
Testing
No human enterprise illustrates our deep-seated sense of superiority
than the use of animals in product and drug testing.
It’s Time (magazine) to Respect
Cows
Animal minds made big news this month with Time magazine dedicating its
cover story to the topic. “Inside the minds of animals,” by Jeffrey
Kluger, provides an engaging glimpse into some of the exciting recent
discoveries in animal cognition. That title is an improvement over that
of a February 2006 Scientific American article which asked meekly, “Do
Animals Have Feelings?” as if there should remain any doubt about that.
Jane GoodAll Things
Wonderful
I dedicate this blog entry to Jane Goodall on the 50th anniversary of
her groundbreaking research on wild chimpanzees.
Whales, Whaling, and
Humanity
In Morocco this week, the International Whaling Commission (IWC) decided
to uphold its international moratorium on commercial whaling. That
decision just might be a litmus test for the evolution and maturity of
humankind. Lifting the ban would have been a symbolic reminder that we
remain mired in a way of thinking that sees other sentient animals
merely as resources for humans to treat as we please-a recapitulation of
an arcane might-makes-right mindset that justified colonialism and
slavery, the subjugation of women, and the denial of civil rights.
Beneath the Surface of an Oil
Spill
As the oil spill in the Gulf of Mexico hemorrhages toward its third
month, there’s a big question about which very little is being said: How
is it making the fish feel? How is the quality of life for individual
fishes being impacted by this latest marine ecological disaster?
Sometimes it hurts when you
duck
I walked into the office of the owner of Hudson Valley Foie Gras (HVFG).
Since opening in 1989, HVFG has become America’s largest producer of the
fatty duck livers treasured by gourmets and reviled by animal advocates.
I wasn’t there to purchase foie gras. I was there on a covert assignment
for PETA … But like slavery and the denial of votes to women before it,
foie gras is vulnerable to exposure as an abomination in civilized
society.
Vulture
Culture
I met a vulture once. She had been on the losing end of a collision with
a car. Unable to fly again, she went on occasional outings where people
could see her up close. I met her at an animal welfare fair in a high
school gymnasium. She was a black vulture, Coragyps atratus. She stood
on a wooden perch, and seemed perfectly at ease in these unnatural
surroundings. She was remarkably unlike my preconceptions of vultures.
She had deep, black eyes. She blinked. She moved her head slowly to
observe the goings on. She wasn’t smelly or scruffy as I had shallowly
imagined vultures to be. She looked immaculate. She had a presence. If I
had to choose one word to describe her it would be dignified. She wasn’t
an object but a subject – a thinking, feeling being.
Papers (with abstracts)
Some of these items have been found through Google Scholar. Certain links are unavailable, but we will try to locate the articles through other means.
Laboratory routines cause animal
stress
Balcombe, Jonathan P.; Barnard, Neal D.; Sandusky, Chad
Eighty published studies were appraised to document the potential stress
associated with three routine laboratory procedures commonly performed
on animals: handling, blood collection, and orogastric gavage. We
defined handling as any non-invasive manipulation occurring as part of
routine husbandry, including lifting an animal and cleaning or moving an
animal’s cage. Significant changes in physiologic parameters correlated
with stress (e.g., serum or plasma concentrations of corticosterone,
glucose, growth hormone or prolactin, heart rate, blood pressure, and
behavior) were associated with all three procedures in multiple species
in the studies we examined. The results of these studies demonstrated
that animals responded with rapid, pronounced, and statistically
significant elevations in stress-related responses for each of the
procedures, although handling elicited variable alterations in immune
system responses. Changes from baseline or control measures typically
ranged from 20% to 100% or more and lasted at least 30 min or longer. We
interpret these findings to indicate that laboratory routines are
associated with stress, and that animals do not readily habituate to
them. The data suggest that significant fear, stress, and possibly
distress are predictable consequences of routine laboratory procedures,
and that these phenomena have substantial scientific and humane
implications for the use of animals in laboratory research.
Vocal recognition of pups by mother Mexican free-tailed bats, Tadarida
brasiliensis
mexicana
JP Balcombe - Animal Behaviour, 1990 - Elsevier
The ability of Mexican free-tailed bat mothers and pups to recognize
vocalizations of their presumptive kin (pup isolation calls and mother
echolocation calls, respectively) was tested using playbacks of recorded
calls. Captive individuals were presented with calls of two bats, one
presumptive kin and the other a stranger, from opposite sides of a
circular wire arena. Response was determined by amount of time spent on
each side of the arena, time spent in contact with a cloth bat model in
front of each speaker, and number of separate contacts with each model.
For the latter two measures, mothers showed a significant preference for
the calls of their presumptive pups. Pups were attracted to adult
echolocation calls, but did not show preference for calls of different
mothers. The ages of pups appears to have had no effect on the
responsiveness of either pups or mothers to the playbacks. This study
demonstrates vocal kin recognition by mothers, and suggests an important
role for acoustic cues in mother-pup reunions in this species. The
findings do not preclude the possibility that vocal recognition of
mothers by pups also occurs.
Vocal recognition in Mexican free-tailed bats: Do pups recognize
mothers?
JP Balcombe, Gary F. McCracken - Animal Behaviour, 1992 - Elsevier
Mother Mexican free-tailed bats, Tadarida brasiliensis mexicana, produce
‘directive’ calls while searching for pups inside cave maternity roosts.
These calls consist of highly repetitive pulses of sound uttered in
rapid sequence. Calls are sufficiently intense that they are perceptible
above the substantial background noise within roosts at distances of at
least 1 m. Calls are stereotyped within individuals, and statistically
discriminable between individuals. These characteristics are expected
for vocalizations that function for mother—pup reunions, and are shared
with directive calls described previously in other bats. Mother T. b.
mexicana directive calls are statistically no less discriminable than
are the isolation calls of pups. Playback experiments, using recordings
made inside the cave colony, show that pups perceive directive calls and
are strongly attracted to them.
Eavesdropping by bats: the influence of echolocation call design and
foraging
strategy
JP Balcombe, M. Brock Fenton - Ethology, 1988 - Wiley Online Library
We used playback presentations to free-flying bats of 3 species to
assess the influence of echolocation call design and foraging strategy
on the role of echolocation calls in communication. Near feeding sites
over water, Myotis lucifugus and M. yumanensis responded positively only
to echolocation calls of conspecifics. Near roosts, these bats did not
respond before young of the year became volant, and after this responded
to presentations of echolocation calls of similar and dissimilar design.
At feeding sites Lasiurus borealis responded only to echolocation calls
of conspecifics and particularly to “feeding buzzes”. While Myotis,
particularly subadults, appear to use the echolocation calls of
conspecifics to locate feeding sites, L. borealis appears to use the
calls of a foraging neighbour attacking prey to identify opportunities
for ‘stealing’ food.
Student/teacher conflict regarding animal
dissection
J Balcombe - The American Biology Teacher, 1997 - JSTOR
IN 1993, a medical student at the University of Colorado was compelled
to transfer to another university when she failed a course for refusing
to participate in a required laboratory exercise that involved
performing lethal procedures on anesthetized dogs.
Animal carcinogenicity studies: 1. Poor human
predictivity
(pdf)
Andrew Knight, Jarrod Bailey and Jonathan Balcombe
The regulation of human exposure to potentially carcinogenic chemicals
constitutes society’s most important use of animal carcinogenicity data.
Environmental contaminants of greatest concern within the USA are listed
in the Environmental Protection Agency’s (EPA’s) Integrated Risk
Information System (IRIS) chemicals database. However, of the 160 IRIS
chemicals lacking even limited human exposure data but possessing animal
data that had received a human carcinogenicity assessment by 1 January
2004, we found that in most cases (58.1%; 93/160), the EPA considered
animal carcinogenicity data inadequate to support a classification of
probable human carcinogen or non-carcinogen. For the 128 chemicals with
human or animal data also assessed by the World Health Organisation’s
International Agency for Research on Cancer (IARC), human
carcinogenicity classifications were compatible with EPA classifications
only for those 17 having at least limited human data (p = 0.5896). For
those 111 primarily reliant on animal data, the EPA was much more likely
than the IARC to assign carcinogenicity classifications indicative of
greater human risk (p < 0.0001). The IARC is a leading international
authority on carcinogenicity assessments, and its significantly
different human carcinogenicity classifications of identical chemicals
indicate that: 1) in the absence of significant human data, the EPA is
over-reliant on animal carcinogenicity data; 2) as a result, the EPA
tends to over-predict carcinogenic risk; and 3) the true predictivity
for human carcinogenicity of animal data is even poorer than is
indicated by EPA figures alone. The EPA policy of erroneously assuming
that tumours in animals are indicative of human carcinogenicity is
implicated as a primary cause of these errors.
The future of teratology research is in
vitro
Bailey, Jarrod; Knight, Andrew; Balcombe, Jonathan
Birth defects induced by maternal exposure to exogenous agents during
pregnancy are preventable, if the agents themselves can be identified
and avoided. Billions of dollars and manhours have been dedicated to
animal-based discovery and characterisation methods over decades. We
show here, via a comprehensive systematic review and analysis of this
data, that these methods constitute questionable science and pose a
hazard to humans. (get pdf)
Animal carcinogenicity studies: 2. Obstacles to extrapolation of data
to
humans
(pdf)
Andrew Knight, Jarrod Bailey and Jonathan Balcombe
Due to limited human exposure data, risk classification and the
consequent regulation of exposure to potential carcinogens has
conventionally relied mainly upon animal tests. However, several
investigations have revealed animal carcinogenicity data to be lacking
in human predictivity. To investigate the reasons for this, we surveyed
160 chemicals possessing animal but not human exposure data within the
US Environmental Protection Agency chemicals database, but which had
received human carcinogenicity assessments by 1 January 2004. We
discovered the use of a wide variety of species, with rodents
predominating, and of a wide variety of routes of administration, and
that there were effects on a particularly wide variety of organ systems.
The likely causes of the poor human predictivity of rodent
carcinogenicity bioassays include: 1) the profound discordance of
bioassay results between rodent species, strains and genders, and
further, between rodents and human beings; 2) the variable, yet
substantial, stresses caused by handling and restraint, and the
stressful routes of administration common to carcinogenicity bioassays,
and their effects on hormonal regulation, immune status and
predisposition to carcinogenesis; differences in rates of absorption and
transport mechanisms between test routes of administration and other
important human routes of exposure; 4) the considerable variability of
organ systems in response to carcinogenic insults, both between and
within species; and 5) the predisposition of chronic high dose bioassays
toward false positive results, due to the overwhelming of physiological
defences, and the unnat- ural elevation of cell division rates during ad
libitum feeding studies. Such factors render profoundly difficult any
attempts to accurately extrapolate human carcinogenic hazards from
animal data.
Alternatives to the use of animals in higher
education (pdf)
Jan van der Valk, David Dewhurst, Ian Hughes, Jeffrey Atkinson, Jonathan
Balcombe, Hans Braun, Karin Gabrielson, Franz Gruber, Jeremy Miles, Jan
Nab, Jason Nardi, Henk van Wilgenburg, Ursula Zinko and Joanne Zurlo
This is the report of the thirty-third of a series of workshops
organised by the European Centre for the Validation of Alternative
Methods (ECVAM). ECVAM’s main goal, as defined in 1993 by its Scientific
Advisory Committee, is to promote the scientific and regulatory
acceptance of alternative methods which are of importance to the
biosciences and which reduce, refine or replace the use of laboratory
animals.
Dissection: The scientific case for
alternatives
J Balcombe - Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 2001 - Taylor &
Francis
This article presents the scientific argument that learning methods that
replace traditional nonhuman animal-consumptive methods in life science
education-so-called alternatives to dissection-are pedagogically sound
and probably superior to dissection. This article focuses on the
pedagogy, a learning method’s effectiveness for conveying knowledge.
Animal carcinogenicity studies: 3. alternatives to the
bioassay
(pdf)
Andrew Knight, Jarrod Bailey and Jonathan Balcombe
Conventional animal carcinogenicity tests take around three years to
design, conduct and interpret. Consequently, only a tiny fraction of the
thousands of industrial chemicals currently in use have been tested for
carcinogenicity. Despite the costs of hundreds of millions of dollars
and millions of skilled personnel hours, as well as millions of animal
lives, several investigations have revealed that animal carcinogenicity
data lack human specificity (i.e. the ability to identify human
non-carcinogens), which severely limits the human predictivity of the
bioassay. This is due to the scientific inadequacies of many
carcinogenicity bioassays, and numerous serious biological obstacles,
which render profoundly difficult any attempts to accurately extrapolate
animal data in order to predict carcinogenic hazards to humans. Proposed
modifications to the conventional bioassays have included the
elimination of mice as a second species, and the use of
genetically-altered or neonatal mice, decreased study durations,
initiation–promotion models, the greater incorporation of toxicokinetic
and toxicodynamic assessments, structure-activity relationship
(computerised) systems, in vitro assays, cDNA microarrays for detecting
changes in gene expression, limited human clinical trials, and
epidemiological research. The potential advantages of non- animal assays
when compared to bioassays include the superior human specificity of the
results, substantially reduced time-frames, and greatly reduced demands
on financial, personnel and animal resources. Inexplicably, however, the
regulatory agencies have been frustratingly slow to adopt alternative
protocols. In order to decrease the enormous cost of cancer to society,
a substantial redirection of resources away from excessively slow and
resource-intensive rodent bioassays, into the further development and
implementation of non-animal assays, is both strongly justified and
urgently required.
Non-breeder asymmetry in Florida scrub
jays
JP Balcombe - Evolutionary Ecology, 1989 - Springer
The data of Woolfenden and Fitzpatrick (1984) show a statistically
significant asymmetry in the sex ratio of non-breeders when one of the
breeders is not the non-breeder’s parent. I propose that the asymmetry
is attributable to a combination of two factors acting on non-breeders:
the value of inheriting a territory, and incest avoidance. Although
natal territories are only occasionally inherited by non-breeders, and
then apparently only by males, therate of inheritance is significantly
higher for parent/step-parent breeders (n=6) than when both breeders are
the non-breeder’s parents (n=1). An alternative hypothesis, that
stepparents determine the non-breeder asymmetry by ousting potential
rivals, might also explain the data, but evidence is currently lacking.
Animal pleasure and its moral
significance
J Balcombe - Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 2009 - Elsevier
This paper presents arguments for, and evidence in support of, the
important role of pleasure in animals’ lives, and outlines its
considerable significance to humankind’s relationship to other animals.
In the realms of animal sentience, almost all scholarly discussion
revolves around its negative aspects: pain, stress, distress, and
suffering. By contrast, the positive aspects of sentience – rewards and
pleasures – have been rarely broached by scientists. Yet, evolutionary
principles predict that animals, like humans, are motivated to seek
rewards, and not merely to avoid pain and suffering. Natural selection
favours behaviours that enhance survival and procreation. In the
conscious, sentient animal, the drives to secure food, shelter, social
contact, and mates are motivated by desire (appetitive behaviour) and
reinforced by pleasure (consummative behaviour). This is reflected in
animals’ behaviour in the realms of play, food, sex, and touch. Despite
the heuristic value of interpreting animal behaviour through the
proximate (experiential) lens, scholarly study of animals remains
entrenched almost exclusively in the ultimate (evolutionary) sphere. Not
just science but also ethics suffer for this, for when we see animals as
only the products of a competitive struggle for survival, we risk
overlooking the positive qualities of their lives. Pleasure has moral
import for such practices as factory farming and laboratory research,
for it amplifies the moral burden of depriving animals the opportunity
to lead fulfilling, enjoyable lives.
Animals Society Courses: A Growing Trend in Post-Secondary
Education
J Balcombe - Society and Animals, 1999 - ingentaconnect.com
A survey of college courses addressing nonhuman animal ethics and
welfare issues indicates that the presence of such courses has increased
greatly since a prior survey was done in 1983. This paper provides
titles and affiliations of 67 of 89 courses from the current survey.
These courses represent 15 academic fields, and a majority are entirely
devoted to animal issues. The fields of animal science and philosophy
are proportionally well represented compared with biology and
wildlife-related fields. An estimated 5000 or more North American
students are now receiving instruction in these issues each year. While
the availability of courses in animal issues is still sporadic, it is
unprecedentedly high and seen as an important component of changing
social values toward nonhuman animals.
Animal carcinogenicity studies: implications for the REACH
system
(pdf)
Andrew Knight, Jarrod Bailey and Jonathan Balcombe
The 2001 European Commission proposal for the Registration, Evaluation
and Authorisation of Chemicals (REACH) aims to improve public and
environmental health by assessing the toxicity of, and restricting
exposure to, potentially toxic chemicals. The greatest benefits are
expected to accrue from decreased cancer incidences. Hence the accurate
identification of chemical carcinogens must be a top priority for the
REACH system. Due to a paucity of human clinical data, the
identification of potential human carcinogens has conventionally relied
on animal tests. However, our survey of the US Environmental Protection
Agency’s (EPA’s) toxic chemicals database revealed that, for a majority
of the chemicals of greatest public health concern (93/160, i.e. 58.1%),
the EPA found animal carcinogenicity data to be inadequate to support
classifications of probable human carcinogen or non-carcinogen. A wide
variety of species were used, with rodents predominating; a wide variety
of routes of administration were used; and a particularly wide variety
of organ systems were affected. These factors raise serious biological
obstacles that render accurate extrapolation to humans profoundly
difficult. Furthermore, significantly different International Agency for
Research on Cancer assessments of identical chemicals, indicate that the
true human predictivity of animal carcinogenicity data is even poorer
than is indicated by the EPA figures alone. Consequently, we propose the
replacement of animal carcinogenicity bioassays with a tiered
combination of non-animal assays, which can be expected to yield a
weight-of-evidence characterisation of carcinogenic risk with superior
human predictivity. Additional advantages include substantial savings of
financial, human and animal resources, and potentially greater insights
into mechanisms of carcinogenicity.
Which drugs cause cancer? For and against: Cancer
bioassays
Andrew Knight, Jarrod Bailey, Jonathan Balcombe
FOR Despite President Nixon’s War on Cancer, launched in 1971, and
billions of dollars spent since then, cancer remains the second-leading
killer of Americans. Around 40% of us will get cancer, and half of us
will die from it. 1 This cease-less tide of human suffering …
Medical training using simulation: Toward fewer animals and safer
patients
(pdf)
J Balcombe - ATLA-NOTTINGHAM-, 2004 - Citeseer
This paper presents the current status of computer-based simulation in
medicine. Recent technological advances have enabled this field to
emerge from esoteric explorations in academic laboratories to
commercially available simulators designed to train users to perform
medical procedures from start to finish. Today, more than a dozen
companies are producing virtual reality simulators and interactive
manikins for training in endoscopy, laparoscopy, anaesthesia, trauma
management, angiography, and needle insertion. For many of these
procedures, thousands of animals are still being used in training. Yet
simulation has many advantages that can transcend scientific, ethical,
economic and logistical problems that arise when using animals. The
first validation studies of medical simulators began appearing in the
late 1990s, and the early results indicate that these devices measure
what they are intended to, and that they can improve performance
relative to traditional learning methods. In addition to expanded use
for new and existing minimally invasive procedures, medical simulators
began appearing in the late 1990s, and the early results indicate that
these devices measure what they are intended to, and that they can
improve performance relative to traditional learning methods. In
addition to expanded use for new and existing minimally invasive
procedures, medical simulators will probably soon be used in physician
credentialing, and they may someday allow surgeons to rehearse
procedures in a patient-specific operating environment. Replacing
animals with simulators in medical training is limited no longer by
technical feasibility but by a willingness of the medical community to
embrace it.
Laboratory rodent welfare: Thinking outside the
cage
J Balcombe - Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 2009 - Taylor &
Francis
This commentary presents the case against housing rats and mice in
laboratory cages; the commentary bases its case on their sentience,
natural history, and the varied detriments of laboratory conditions. The
commentary gives 5 arguments to support this position: (a) rats and mice
have a high degree of sentience and can suffer, (b) laboratory
environments cause suffering, (c) rats and mice in the wild have
discrete behavioral needs, (d) rats and mice bred for many generations
in the laboratory retain these needs, and (e) these needs are not met in
laboratory cages.
Verbal Vivisection
J Balcombe - Organization & environment, 2000 - oae.sagepub.com
This commentary revisits and provides some further analysis on Dunayer’s
critique of language use by the animal research community. Without
question, such language is often used to distort and soften certain
harsh realities of vivisection. The commentator elaborates on the terms
consent, sacrifice, euthanasia, and vivisection and reflects on the
particular challenges faced by any writer who sets out to assail the use
of language by another interest group. For in doing so, the writer risks
falling into the same traps of manipulative or prejudicial language that
are being denounced. Dunayer is not immune to this, but she raises some
valid and provocative points along the way.
Animal
Dissection
J Balcombe - 1998 - eric.ed.gov
Killing animals for classroom dissection causes animal suffering,
cheapens the value of life, and depletes wild animal populations, yet it
remains commonplace. The Humane Society of the United States (HSUS)
addresses the issue in this information packet which includes a fact
sheet and three resource lists “on Dissection.” The fact sheet discusses
the numbers of animals killed for dissection in schools, kinds of
animals used, sources of animals, industry methods, lack of industry
oversight, student feelings, legislation, available alternatives,
educational pros and cons, and suggestions for student action. An
annotated list of studies on attitudes toward dissection includes 13
studies of student attitudes from elementary school through medical
school. A second annotated bibliography addresses comparative studies of
dissection and other animal uses in education. These studies include
comparisons of student performance between groups performing dissection
and those learning using models, computer simulation, lecture, or
sequential slides. The studies also review field-based animal research
as opposed to laboratory-based studies of animal behavior.
Intelligent, social rat can find joy in a hostile
world
J Balcombe - Nature, 2005 - nature.com
One has to admire the guile and tenacity of the lone rat described by
James Russell and colleagues (“Intercepting the first rat ashore”,
Nature 437, 1107; 2005). This rat lived for ten weeks on a small,
booby-trapped island, visited by trained rat-killing dogs, before
swimming a quarter of a mile to a neighbouring island, where he survived
a further two months of concerted efforts to eliminate him.
EuroNICHE 10th Anniversary
Conference
J Balcombe - Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 1998 - Taylor &
Francis
The European Network of Individuals and Campaigns for Humane Education
(EuroNICHE) held its 10th conference November 28 through 30 in Hoek van
Holland, The Netherlands. EuroNICHE describes itself as" an
international network of students, teachers, and other …
Laboratory animals deserve better legal
protection
J Balcombe - Nature Medicine, 2008 - nature.com
Given that she is head of the National Association for Biomedical
Research, Frankie Trull’s professed concern for animal welfare in her
interview is disingenuous considering her organization’s history of
campaigning to ensure that rats, mice and birds be excluded from Animal
Welfare Act (AWA) protections. The idea that these animals are not
worthy of AWA coverage is unscientific and unethical. Rats and mice, in
particular, continue to be exposed to such torments as inescapable
electric shocks and force-feedings. Are these really what anyone would
consider “the most humane conditions”?
Dissection: The Scientific Case for
Alternatives
(pdf)
J Balcombe - 2001 - my.psychologytoday.com
This article presents the scientific argument that leaming methods that
replace tradi-tional nonhuman animal-consumptive methods in life science
education——so-called alternatives to dissection-are pedagogically sound
and probably superior to dissection.
ECVAM Workshop on Alternatives to the Use of Animals in Higher
Education
J Balcombe - Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 1999 - Taylor &
Francis
The movement for alternatives to the use of animals in laboratories has
focused primarily on testing and research and relatively little on
education, yet educational use of animals is substantial-numbering
annually many millions of animals worldwide. The potential for …
Humane Science Projects: Suggestions for Biology
Studies
Balcombe, Jonathan P., Comp.
This paper lists 35 studies in biology which can be tailored to suit the
full range of student age groups and are designed to involve most or all
of the key elements of the scientific process (study design, data
collection and presentation, and experimental manipulation). Examples of
some studies are: (1) study the growth of molds on food items under
different growing conditions; vary foods and growing conditions; (2)
study absenteeism in school; relate to colds, flu, other illnesses; (3)
visit a local pond where bats forage at dusk; time of arrival of the
bats on different nights and compare with time of year; estimate insect
abundance by counting sudden changes of flight direction; and (4)
conduct a behavioral study of your companion animal(s) at home: e.g., to
what sounds do they respond; compare response to different vocal
inflections; examine play behavior, etc. (get pdf)
Toward Genuine Rodent Welfare: Response to Reviewer
Comments
J Balcombe - Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 2009 - Taylor &
Francis
I’m grateful to the editors for soliciting critiques of my commentary
and for the opportunity to respond. Because one of the respondents
(Patterson-Kane, 2010 12. Patterson-Kane, E. 2010/this issue. Thinking
outside of our cages. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science …
Self-Harm in Laboratory-Housed Primates: Where Is the Evidence That the
Animal Welfare Act Amendment Has
Worked?
J Balcombe, H Ferdowsian,D Durham - Journal of Applied Animal Welfare
Science, 2011 - Taylor & Francis
The 1985 amendment to the United States Animal Welfare Act (AWA) to
promote psychological well being of primates in the laboratory
represents an acknowledgment of an important welfare problem concerning
nonhuman animals. How effective has this amendment been? Perhaps the
best-known contributor to psychological distress in primates in the
laboratory is nonsocial housing; yet, available analyses suggest that
little progress has been made in avoiding single-caging of these
animals. Another way to assess psychological well being is to examine
rates of self-abusive behavior in laboratory primates. If the AWA has
been effective, then post-AWA self-harm rates might be lower than
pre-AWA rates. However, when we attempted to determine those rates from
published studies, data were too sparse to allow a rigorous statistical
analysis; of 139 studies reporting primate self-harming behavior, only 9
contained data allowing estimation of self-harming behavior rates. We
conclude that the current system of laboratory animal care and record
keeping is inadequate to properly assess AWA impacts on primate
psychological well being and that more is required to ensure the
psychological well being of primates.